In linguistic morphology, inflection (less commonly, inflexion) is a process of word formationDavid Crystal. (2008). A Dictionary of Linguistics and Phonetics (6th ed., pp. 243–244). Malden, MA: Blackwell. in which a word is modified to express different grammatical categories such as tense, grammatical case, voice, aspect, person, number, gender, grammatical mood, animacy, and definiteness. The inflection of is called conjugation, while the inflection of , , , etc. can be called declension.
An inflection expresses grammatical categories with (such as prefix, suffix, infix, circumfix, and transfix), apophony (as Indo-European ablaut), or other modifications. For example, the Latin verb ducam, meaning "I will lead", includes the suffix -am, expressing person (first), number (singular), and tense-mood (future indicative or present subjunctive). The use of this suffix is an inflection. In contrast, in the English clause "I will lead", the word lead is not inflected for any of person, number, or tense; it is simply the bare infinitive of a verb. The inflected form of a word often contains both one or more (a unit of meaning which can stand by itself as a word), and one or more (a unit of meaning which cannot stand alone as a word). For example, the English word cars is a noun that is inflected for number, specifically to express the plural; the content morpheme car is unbound because it could stand alone as a word, while the suffix -s is bound because it cannot stand alone as a word. These two morphemes together form the inflected word cars.
Words that are never subject to inflection are said to be invariant; for example, the English verb must is an invariant item: it never takes a suffix or changes form to signify a different grammatical category. Its categories can be determined only from its context. Languages that seldom make use of inflection, such as English language, are said to be analytic. Analytic languages that do not make use of derivational morphemes, such as Standard Chinese, are said to be isolating.
Requiring the forms or inflections of more than one word in a sentence to be compatible with each other according to the rules of the language is known as concord or agreement. For example, in "the man jumps", "man" is a singular noun, so "jump" is constrained in the present tense to use the third person singular suffix "s".
Languages that have some degree of inflection are synthetic languages. They can be highly inflected (such as Georgian or Kichwa language), moderately inflected (such as Russian language or Latin), weakly inflected (such as English language), but not uninflected (such as Chinese language). Languages that are so inflected that a sentence can consist of a single highly inflected word (such as many Native American languages) are called polysynthetic languages. Languages in which each inflection conveys only a single grammatical category, such as Finnish language, are known as agglutinative languages, while languages in which a single inflection can convey multiple grammatical roles (such as both nominative case and plural, as in Latin and German language) are called fusional.
There are eight regular inflectional affixes in the English language.
+Inflectional affixes in English !Affix !Grammatical category !Mark !Part of speech | |||
-s | Number | plural | |
-'s/'/s | Grammatical case | Genitive case | and , pronouns (marks independent genitive) |
-ing | Aspect | progressive | or |
-en/-ed | Aspect | perfect | |
-ed/-t | Tense | Past tense (Simple aspect) | |
-s | Person, number, aspect, tense | 3rd person singular present Indicative mood | |
-er | Degree of comparison | comparative | and adverbs |
-est | Degree of comparison | superlative | and adverbs |
For details, see English plural, English verbs, and English irregular verbs.
For instance, many languages that feature verb inflection have both regular verbs and irregular verbs. In English, regular verbs form their past tense and past participle with the ending -ed. Therefore, verbs like play, arrive and enter are regular, while verbs like sing, keep and go are irregular. Irregular verbs often preserve patterns that were regular in past forms of the language, but which have now become anomalous; in rare cases, there are regular verbs that were irregular in past forms of the language. (For more details see English verbs and English irregular verbs.)
Other types of irregular inflected form include irregular plural nouns, such as the English mice, children and women (see English plural) and the French yeux (the plural of œil, "eye"); and irregular comparative and superlative forms of adjectives or adverbs, such as the English better and best (which correspond to the positive form good or well).
Irregularities can have four basic causes:
For more details on some of the considerations that apply to regularly and irregularly inflected forms, see the article on regular and irregular verbs.
An organized list of the inflected forms of a given lexeme or root word is called its declension if it is a noun, or its conjugation if it is a verb.
Below is the declension of the English pronoun I, which is inflected for case and number.
! singular ! plural | ||
Subject pronoun | I | we |
Object pronoun | me | us |
possessive determiner | my | our |
possessive pronoun | mine | ours |
reflexive | myself | ourselves |
The pronoun who is also inflected according to case. Its declension is Defective verb, in the sense that it lacks a reflexive form.
! singular and plural | |
nominative | who |
oblique | whom (traditional), who (informal) |
possessive | whose |
reflexive | – |
The following table shows the conjugation of the verb to arrive in the indicative grammatical mood: suffixes inflect it for person, number, and tense:
The Non-finite verb arriv e (bare infinitive), arrived (past participle) and arriv ing (gerund/present participle), although not inflected for person or number, can also be regarded as part of the conjugation of the verb to arrive. Compound verb, such as I have arrived, I had arrived, or I will arrive, can be included also in the conjugation of the verb for didactic purposes, but they are not overt inflections of arrive. The formula for deriving the covert form, in which the relevant inflections do not occur in the main verb, is
engel 'angel' | scip 'ship' | sorg 'sorrow' | |||
Nominative | sorg a | ||||
sorg a/sorg e | |||||
sorg a | |||||
sorg um | |||||
nama 'name' | ēage 'eye' | tunge 'tongue' | |||
tung an | |||||
tung an | |||||
tung ena | |||||
tung um |
The terms "strong declension" and "weak declension" are primarily relevant to well-known dependent-marking languages (such as the Indo-European languages, or Japanese). In dependent-marking languages, nouns in adpositional (prepositional or postpositional) phrases can carry inflectional morphemes.
In head-marking languages, the adpositions can carry the inflection in adpositional phrases. This means that these languages will have inflected adpositions. In Western Apache (San Carlos dialect), the postposition -ká’ 'on' is inflected for person and number with prefixes:
! colspan="2" Singular ! colspan="2" | Dual ! colspan="2" | Plural |
Traditional grammars have specific terms for inflections of nouns and verbs but not for those of .
In contrast, derivation is the process of adding derivational morphemes, which create a new word from existing words and change the semantic meaning or the part of speech of the affected word, such as by changing a noun to a verb.
Distinctions between verbal moods are mainly indicated by derivational morphemes.
Words are rarely listed in dictionaries on the basis of their inflectional morphemes (in which case they would be lexical items). However, they often are listed on the basis of their derivational morphemes. For instance, English dictionaries list readable and readability, words with derivational suffixes, along with their root read. However, no traditional English dictionary lists book as one entry and books as a separate entry; the same goes for jump and jumped.
+ Inflectional Reduplication |
buku-buku 'books' |
ren24 ren24 'everyone' |
ang24 ang24 'reddish' |
ag-basbása 'reading' |
gohu-gohu 'getting dark' |
baabazu’ 'be washing' |
+ Verb paradigm of 'bend' in Tlatepuzco Chinantec ! scope="col" | ! scope="col" 1 SG ! scope="col" | 1 PL ! scope="col" | 2 ! scope="col" | 3 |
Case can be distinguished with tone as well, as in Maasai language (a Nilo-Saharan language spoken in Kenya and Tanzania) (Hyman, 2016):
+ Case Inflection in Maasai ! scope="col" | gloss ! scope="col" | Nominative ! scope="col" | Accusative |
In comparison, Icelandic preserves almost all of the inflections of Old Norse and remains heavily inflected. It retains all the grammatical cases from Old Norse and is inflected for number and three different grammatical genders. The dual number forms are however almost completely lost in comparison to Old Norse.
Unlike other Germanic languages, nouns are inflected for definiteness in all Scandinavian languages, like in the following case for Nynorsk:
+Inflection of nouns in Norwegian (nynorsk) ! rowspan="2" | ! colspan="2"Singular ! colspan="2" | Plural | |
a car | the car | cars | the cars |
a wagon | the wagon | wagons | the wagons |
a house | the house | houses | the houses |
+Articles in Norwegian (nynorsk) ! rowspan="2" | ! colspan="2"Singular ! colspan="2" | Plural |
Latin, the mother tongue of the Romance languages, was highly inflected; nouns and adjectives had different forms according to seven (including five major ones) with five major patterns of declension, and three genders instead of the two found in most Romance tongues. There were four patterns of conjugation in six tenses, three moods (indicative, subjunctive, imperative, plus the infinitive, participle, gerund, gerundive, and supine) and two voices (passive and active), all overtly expressed by affixes (passive voice forms were periphrastic in three tenses).
Dual form is obsolete in standard Latvian and nowadays it is also considered nearly obsolete in standard Lithuanian. For instance, in standard Lithuanian it is normal to say "dvi varnos (plural) – two crows" instead of "dvi varni (dual)". Adjectives, pronouns, and numerals are declined for number, gender, and case to agree with the noun they modify or for which they substitute. Baltic verbs are inflected for tense, mood, aspect, and voice. They agree with the subject in person and number (not in all forms in modern Latvian).
Declensional endings depend on case (nominative, genitive, dative, accusative, locative, instrumental, vocative), number (singular, dual or plural), gender (masculine, feminine, neuter) and animacy (animate vs inanimate). Unusual in other language families, declension in most Slavic languages also depends on whether the word is a noun or an adjective. Slovene and Sorbian languages use a rare third number, (in addition to singular and plural numbers) known as dual (in case of some words dual survived also in Polish language and other Slavic languages). Modern Russian, Serbian and Czech also use a more complex form of dual, but this misnomer applies instead to numbers 2, 3, 4, and larger numbers ending in 2, 3, or 4 (with the exception of the teens, which are handled as plural; thus, 102 is dual, but 12 or 127 are not). In addition, in some Slavic languages, such as Polish, word stems are frequently modified by the addition or absence of endings, resulting in Apophony.
Arabic regional dialects (e.g. Morocco Arabic, Arabic, Persian Gulf Arabic), used for everyday communication, tend to have less inflection than the more formal Literary Arabic. For example, in Arabic, the second- and third-person feminine plurals (أنتنّ and هنّ ) and their respective unique conjugations are lost and replaced by the masculine (أنتم and هم ), whereas in Lebanese and Syrian Arabic, هم is replaced by هنّ .
In addition, the system known as Irab places vowel suffixes on each verb, noun, adjective, and adverb, according to its function within a sentence and its relation to surrounding words.
Hungarian and Finnish, in particular, often simply concatenate suffixes. For example, Finnish talossanikinko "in my house, too?" consists of talo-ssa-ni-kin-ko. However, in the Finnic languages (Finnish, Estonian etc.) and the Sami languages, there are processes which affect the root, particularly consonant gradation. The original suffixes may disappear (and appear only by liaison), leaving behind the modification of the root. This process is extensively developed in Estonian and Sami, and makes them also inflected, not only agglutinating languages. The Estonian illative case, for example, is expressed by a modified root: maja → majja (historical form * maja-han).
Noun phrase morphology is agglutinative and consists of suffixes which simply attach to the end of a stem (and more generally, only once at the very end of the nominal syntagma). These suffixes are in many cases fused with the article ( -a for singular and -ak for plural), which in general is required to close a noun phrase in Basque if no other determiner is present, and unlike an article in many languages, it can only partially be correlated with the concept of definiteness. Proper nouns do not take an article, and indefinite nouns without the article (called mugagabe in Basque grammar) are highly restricted syntactically. Basque is an ergative language, meaning that inflectionally the single argument (subject) of an intransitive verb is marked in the same way as the direct object of a transitive verb. This is called the absolutive case and in Basque, as in most ergative languages, it is realized with a zero morph; in other words, it receives no special inflection. The subject of a transitive verb receives a special case suffix, called the ergative case.King, Alan R. The Basque Language: A Practical Introduction. University of Nevada Press. Reno, Nevada
There is no case marking concord in Basque and case suffixes, including those fused with the article, are added only to the last word in a noun phrase. Plurality is not marked on the noun and is identified only in the article or other determiner, possibly fused with a case marker. The examples below are in the absolutive case with zero case marking, and include the article only:
(the/a) dog |
(the) dogs |
(the/a) pretty dog |
(the) pretty dogs |
The noun phrase is declined for 11 cases: Absolutive, ergative, dative, possessive-genitive, benefactive, comitative, instrumental, inessive, allative, ablative, and local-genitive. These are signaled by suffixes that vary according to the categories of Singular, Plural, Indefinite, and Proper Noun, and many vary depending on whether the stem ends in a consonant or vowel. The Singular and Plural categories are fused with the article, and these endings are used when the noun phrase is not closed by any other determiner. This gives a potential 88 different forms, but the Indefinite and Proper Noun categories are identical in all but the local cases (inessive, allative, ablative, local-genitive), and many other variations in the endings can be accounted for by phonological rules operating to avoid impermissible consonant clusters. Local case endings are not normally added to animate Proper Nouns. The precise meaning of the local cases can be further specified by additional suffixes added after the local case suffixes.
Verb forms are extremely complex, agreeing with the subject, direct object, and indirect object; and include forms that agree with a "dative of interest" for intransitive verbs as well as allocutive forms where the verb form is altered if one is speaking to a close acquaintance. These allocutive forms also have different forms depending on whether the addressee is male or female. This is the only area in Basque grammar where gender plays any role at all. Subordination could also plausibly be considered an inflectional category of the Basque verb since subordination is signaled by prefixes and suffixes on the conjugated verb, further multiplying the number of potential forms.Manandise, Esméralda. "Evidence from Basque for a New Theory of Grammar", doctoral dissertation in Outstanding Dissertations in Linguistics: A Garland Series, Jorge Hankamer, general ed. Garland Publishing, Inc. New York & London.
Transitivity is a thoroughgoing division of Basque verbs, and it is necessary to know the transitivity of a particular verb in order to conjugate it successfully. In the spoken language only a handful of commonly used verbs are fully conjugated in the present and simple past, most verbs being conjugated by means of an auxiliary which differs according to transitivity. The literary language includes a few more such verbs, but the number is still very small. Even these few verbs require an auxiliary to conjugate other tenses besides the present and simple past.
The most common intransitive auxiliary is izan, which is also the verb for "to be". The most common transitive auxiliary is ukan, which is also the verb for "to have". (Other auxiliaries can be used in some of the tenses and may vary by dialect.) The compound tenses use an invariable form of the main verb (which appears in different forms according to the "tense group") and a conjugated form of the auxiliary. Pronouns are normally omitted if recoverable from the verb form. A couple of examples will have to suffice to demonstrate the complexity of the Basque verb:
The morphs that represent the various tense/person/case/mood categories of Basque verbs, especially in the auxiliaries, are so highly fused that segmenting them into individual meaningful units is nearly impossible, if not pointless. Considering the multitude of forms that a particular Basque verb can take, it seems unlikely that an individual speaker would have an opportunity to utter them all in his or her lifetime.Manandise, Esméralda. "Evidence from Basque for a New Theory of Grammar", doctoral dissertation in Outstanding Dissertations in Linguistics: A Garland Series, Jorge Hankamer, general ed. Garland Publishing, Inc. New York & London.
Both sentences mean 'The boy sees the girl.' This is because puer (boy) is singular nominative, puellam (girl) is singular accusative. Since the roles of puer and puellam have been marked with case endings, the change in position does not matter.
The situation is very different in Chinese. Since Modern Chinese makes no use of inflection, the meanings of wǒ ('I' or 'me') and tā ('he' or 'him') shall be determined with their position.
In Classical Chinese, pronouns were overtly inflected to mark case. However, these overt case forms are no longer used; most of the alternative pronouns are considered archaic in modern Mandarin Chinese. Classically, 我 ( wǒ) was used solely as the first person accusative. 吾 ( Wú) was generally used as the first person nominative.Norman, Jerry. (1988). Chinese (p. 98). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Certain varieties of Chinese are known to express meaning by means of tone change, although further investigations are required. Note that the tone change must be distinguished from tone sandhi. Tone sandhi is a compulsory change that occurs when certain tones are juxtaposed. Tone change, however, is a morphologically conditioned alternation and is used as an inflectional or a derivational strategy. Examples from Taishanese and Zhongshan (both Yue Chinese spoken in Guangdong Province) are shown below:Chen, M. Y. (2000). Tone Sandhi: Patterns across Chinese dialects. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
‘I’ (singular) |
‘we’ (plural) |
‘go’ |
‘gone’ (perfective) |
The following table compares the personal pronouns of Sixian dialect (a dialect of Taiwanese Hakka) with Zaiwa and Jingpho (both Tibeto-Burman languages spoken in Yunnan and Myanmar). The superscripted numbers indicate the Tone letter.
+ Comparison of Personal Pronouns ! scope="col" | ! scope="col" Sixian dialect ! scope="col" | Zaiwa language ! scope="col" | Jingpho language |
In Shanghainese, the third-person singular pronoun is overtly inflected as to case and the first- and second-person singular pronouns exhibit a change in tone depending on case.
Nouns are marked for number (singular and plural), and the accusative case may be shown in certain situations, typically when the direct object of a sentence precedes its verb. On the other hand, adjectives are unmarked for gender, number or case (unless they stand on their own, without a noun, in which case they take on the same desinences as the missing noun would have taken). The definite article "la" ("the") remains unaltered regardless of gender or case, and also of number, except when there is no other word to show plurality. Pronouns are identical in all cases, though exceptionally the accusative case may be marked, as for nouns.
|
|