An ideophone (also known as a mimetic or expressive) is a member of the word class that depict sensory imagery or sensations, evoking ideas of action, sound, movement, color, or shape. The class of ideophones is the least common syntactic category cross-linguistically; it occurs mostly in African, Australian, and Amerindian languages, and sporadically elsewhere. Ideophones resemble but are different owing to their special Phonetics or derivational characteristics, and based on their syntactic function within the sentence. They may include sounds that deviate from the language's phonological system, Imitation—often in a repetitive manner—sounds of movement, animal noises, bodily sounds, noises made by tools or machines, and the like.
While English language does have ideophonic or onomatopoetic expressions, it does not contain a proper class of ideophones because any English onomatopoeic word can be included in one of the classical categories. For example, la-di-da functions as an adjective while others, such as zigzag, may function as a verb, adverb or adjective, depending on the clausal context. In the sentence "The rabbit zigzag ged across the meadow", the verb zigzag takes the past -ed verb ending. In contrast, the reconstructed example *"The rabbit zigzag zigzag across the meadow" emulates an ideophone but is not idiomatic to English.
Dictionaries of languages like Japanese, Korean, Xhosa, Yoruba, and Zulu list thousands of ideophones. Sometimes ideophones are called phonosemantic to indicate that it is not a grammatical word class in the traditional sense of the word (like verb or noun), but rather a lexical class based on the special relationship between form and meaning exhibited by ideophones. In the discipline of linguistics, ideophones have sometimes been overlooked or treated as a subgroup of interjections.
Characteristics
The word ideophone was coined in 1935 by Clement Martyn Doke, who defined it in his
Bantu Linguistic Terminology as follows.
[Doke 1935 as cited in Voeltz & Kilian-Hatz 2001]
Ideophones evoke sensory events. A well known instance of ideophones are onomatopoeia words—words that imitate the sound (of the event) they refer to. Some ideophones may be derived from onomatopoeic notions. In many languages, however, ideophones do not solely represent sound. For instance, in Gbaya languages, kpuk 'a rap on the door' may be onomatopoeic, but other ideophones depict motion and visual scenes: loɓoto-loɓoto 'large animals plodding through mud', kiláŋ-kiláŋ 'in a zigzagging motion', pɛɗɛŋ-pɛɗɛŋ 'razor sharp'.
Ideophones are often characterized as Iconicity or Sound symbolism words, meaning that there can be a resemblance between their form and their meaning. For instance, in West-African languages, voiced consonants and low tone in ideophones are often connected to largeness and heaviness, whereas voiceless consonants and high tones tend to relate to smallness and lightness.[Westermann 1927] Reduplication figures quite prominently in ideophones, often conveying a sense of repetition or plurality present in the evoked event.[Watson in Voeltz & Kilian-Hatz 2001] The iconicity of ideophones is shown by the fact that people can guess the meanings of ideophones from various languages at a level above chance.[Iwasaki et al. 2007, Dingemanse et al. 2015] However, the form of ideophones does not completely relate to their meaning; as conventionalized words, they contain arbitrary, language-specific phonemes just like other parts of the vocabulary.
Grammar
The grammatical function of ideophones varies by language. In some languages (e.g.
Welayta language, Yir-Yiront,
Semai language,
Korean language), they form a separate word class, while in others, they occur across a number of different word classes (e.g.
Mundang,
Ewe language,
Sotho language,
Hausa language).
[Voeltz & Kilian-Hatz 2001]
Despite this diversity, ideophones show a number of robust regularities across languages. One is that they are often marked in the same way as quoted speech and demonstrations. Sometimes ideophones can form a complete utterance on their own, as in English "ta-da!" or Japanese ta-da.[Diffloth 1972] However, in such cases the word ideophone is used as a synonym to interjection. Proper ideophones may occur within utterances, depicting a scene described by other elements of the utterance, as in Japanese Taro wa sutasuta to haya-aruki o shita "Taro walked hurriedly' (literally 'Taro did haste-walk sutasuta'). Ideophones are more like illustrations of events than responses to events. An ideophone like Gbaya kiláŋ-kiláŋ 'in a zigzagging motion' displays a certain resemblance to the event (for instance, its irregular vowels and tones depicting the irregularity of the motion).
Registers
Languages may differ in the context in which ideophones are used. In some languages, ideophones are primarily used in spoken language (e.g.
narrative contexts) and are rarely encountered in written language.
[Noss in Voeltz & Kilian-Hatz 2001] In other languages (e.g.
Ewe language, Japanese), ideophones can be freely used in all registers. In general, however, ideophones tend to occur more extensively in spoken language because of their expressive or dramaturgic function.
[Kunene in Voeltz & Kilian-Hatz 2001]
Examples
Japanese
The Japanese language has thousands of ideophones, often called
mimetics. The constructions are quite metrical 2-2, or 3-3, where morae play a role in the symmetry. The first consonant of the second word of the reduplication
rendaku if phonological conditions allow. Japanese ideophones are used extensively in daily conversations as well as in the written language.
-
doki doki (ドキドキ) – heart-pounding
-
kira kira (キラキラ) – glittery
-
shiin (シーン) – silence
-
niko niko (ニコニコ) – smile
-
jii (じー) – stare
-
run run (ルンルン) – cheerful
tamil_la">
[[Tamil|Tamil language
The Tamil language uses many ideophones, both in spoken (colloquial) and in formal usage. Ideophones are called irattaik kilavi (இரட்டைக் கிளவி) in Tamil grammar.
-
sora sora (சொறசொற) – rough (the sound produced when rubbing back and forth on a rough surface)
-
vazha-vazha (வழவழ) – smooth, slippery
-
mozhu-mozhu (மொழுமொழு) – smooth (surface)
-
kozhu-kozhu (கொழுகொழு) – plump
-
kozha-kozha (கொழகொழ) – slimy, gooey
-
busu-busu (புசுபுசு) – soft and bushy
-
kaNa-kaNa (கணகண) - warm, hot
-
giDu-giDu (கிடுகிடு) – quickly, fast
-
Tak-Tak (டக்டக்) - quickly, rapidly
-
maDa-maDa (மடமட) – quickly, fast
-
masa-masa (மசமச) – sluggish, lethargic
-
viru-viru (விறுவிறு) – energetically (also, spicy)
-
choda-choda (சொதசொத) – marshy, waterlogged
-
paLa-paLa (பளப்பள/பளபள) – glittering, shiny
-
veDa-veDa (வெடவெட) – shaking, trembling
-
chuDa-chuDa (சுடச்சுட) – piping hot
-
mAngu-mAngu (மாங்குமாங்கு) – laboriously
-
gara-gara (கரகர) – crunchy (as in food), gravely (as in voice)
-
gaba-gaba (கபகப) - wolfing down food
-
doLa-doLa (தொளதொள) - hanging loose (as in loose fitting)
-
taLa-taLa (தளதள) - lush (as in a lush plant/orchard)
-
toNa-toNa (தொணதொண) - annoyingly incessant
Xhosa
In
Xhosa language, as in closely related Zulu, ideophones can convey very complex experiential impressions or can just strengthen meanings of other words. The ideophone is often introduced using the verb thi .
Using thi:
- : Lixesha lokuthi cwaka. 'It is time to be silent.' literally:
- : Bathi gqi abelungu eAfrika. 'The white people suddenly arrived in Africa.' Literally:)
Without using thi:
-
bhuxe – to stand motionless
See also
Bibliography