Product Code Database
Example Keywords: mmorpg -netbooks $81-150
   » » Wiki: Aphantasia
Tag Wiki 'Aphantasia'.
Tag

Aphantasia ( , ) is the inability to voluntarily visualize mental images.

(2025). 9783319298214, Springer. .

The phenomenon was first described by in 1880, but it has remained relatively unstudied. Interest in the phenomenon was renewed after the publication of a study in 2015 by a team led by the neurologist Adam Zeman of the University of Exeter. Zeman's team coined the term aphantasia, derived from the word phantasia (), which means 'appearance/image', and the prefix a- (), which means 'without'. People with aphantasia are called aphantasics, or less commonly aphants or aphantasiacs.

Aphantasia can be considered the opposite of , the condition of having extremely vivid .

(2021). 9780128213773


History
The phenomenon was first described by in 1880, in a statistical study on . Galton wrote:

In 1897, the psychologist Théodule-Armand Ribot reported a kind of "typographic visual type" imagination, consisting in mentally seeing ideas in the form of the corresponding printed words.

(1897). 9782296023345, L'Harmattan.
As paraphrased by ,

The phenomenon remained largely unstudied until 2005, when Professor Adam Zeman of the University of Exeter was approached by a man who seemed to have lost the ability to visualize after undergoing minor surgery. Following the publication of this patient's case in 2010, a number of people approached Zeman reporting a lifelong inability to visualize. In 2015, Zeman's team published a paper on what they termed " aphantasia"—a form of aphantasia in which individuals have never had the ability to generate voluntary mental images— sparking renewed interest in the phenomenon.

The idea of aphantasia was popularised on social media in 2020, through posts which asked the reader to imagine a red apple and rate their "mind's eye" depiction of it on a scale from 1 (photographic visualisation) through to 5 (no visualisation at all). Many people were shocked to learn that their own ability or inability to visualise objects was not universal.


Research
Zeman's 2015 paper used the Vividness of Visual Imagery Questionnaire (VVIQ), developed by David Marks in 1973, to evaluate the quality of the of 21 self-diagnosed and self-selected participants. He found that most aphantasics lack voluntary visualizations only; the majority of test subjects did report involuntary visualizations such as . Along with Zeman's reports of involuntary mental imagery occurring during dream states, a 2020 study found that individuals with aphantasia experience less control and fewer sensory emotions during dreams compared to those with a strong ability to generate voluntary mental images while awake. The lack of intense emotions is thought to result from a functional trade-off—competition between two neural systems—occurring between semantic information and sensory qualities, which is strongly associated with individuals who have low VVIQ scores.

In 2017, a paper measured the sensory capacity of mental imagery using binocular-rivalry (BR) and imagery-based priming and found that when asked to imagine a stimulus, the self-reported aphantasics experienced almost no perceptual priming, compared to those who reported higher imagery scores where perceptual priming had an effect. In 2020, Keogh and Pearson published another paper illustrating measurable differences correlated with visual imagery, this time by indirectly measuring cortical excitability in the primary (V1).

In 2018, a study analyzing the visual working memory of a person with aphantasia found that mental imagery has a "functional role in areas of visual cognition, one of which is high-precision working memory" and that the person with aphantasia performed significantly worse than controls on visual working memory trials requiring the highest degree of precision, and lacked metacognitive insight into their performance.

A 2020 study concluded that those who experience aphantasia also experience reduced imagery in other senses, and have less vivid autobiographical memories. In addition to deficits in autobiographical memories compared to people without aphantasia, people with aphantasia had significant differences in all aspects of memory when compared to the performance of people without aphantasia. A 2021 study concluded that while those with aphantasia reported fewer objects in drawing recall, they showed high spatial memory concerning controls in drawings, with these differences only appearing during the recall stage of the study.

In 2021, a study by Keogh, Wicken, and Pearson focusing on the role of visual imagery in visual working memory tasks specifically considered the strategies people with aphantasia use in these tasks. It found no significant differences in visual working memory task performances for those with aphantasia when compared to controls. However, significant differences were found in the reported strategies used by aphantasic individuals across the memory tasks.

In 2021, a study that measured the perspiration (via skin conductance levels) of participants in response to reading a frightening story and then viewing fear-inducing images found that participants with aphantasia, but not the general population, experienced a flat-line physiological response during the reading experiment, but found no difference in physiological responses between the groups when participants viewed fear-inducing images. The study concluded the evidence supported the emotional amplification theory of visual imagery.

In 2021, a study found that people with aphantasia have slower reaction times than people without aphantasia in a in which they were presented with a target and a distractor. But both groups saw a similar reduction in reaction time when primed with the color of the target compared to if primed with the color of the distractor or a third color, suggesting that people with and without aphantasia were primed in the same way. The researchers hypothesized that this may be because the color of the prime is not relevant to the search task. To explore this, a follow-up experiment by the same researchers found people without aphantasia saw a greater reduction in reaction time when selecting the target from two images compared to from two words. At the same time, both people with and without aphantasia were faster in the image task than the word task. A 2023 study explored more natural scenarios and found that aphantasics are slower at solving hidden object pictures.

In 2021, a study relating aphantasia, , and was published that found that people with aphantasia reported more autistic traits than people without aphantasia, with weaknesses in imagination and social skills.

In addition to congenital aphantasia, there have been cases reported of acquired aphantasia—characterized by new onset of diminished voluntary visual imagery—due either to brain injury or psychological causes. In 2021, a study reported on acquired aphantasia following a case of COVID-19.

A 2021 study aimed to provide insights into the correlation between auditory and visual imagery. The research, conducted on a sample of 128 participants, included 34 individuals who self-identified as having aphantasia. The study found a strong association between auditory imagery (measured using the Bucknell Auditory Imagery Scale-Vividness, BAIS-V) and visual imagery (measured using the Vividness of Visual Imagery Questionnaire-Modified, VVIQ-M). They found most people who self-reported having aphantasia also reported weak or entirely absent auditory imagery. Moreover, participants lacking auditory imagery tended to be aphantasic. The authors proposed a new term, "anauralia", to describe the absence of auditory imagery, particularly the lack of an "inner voice". A subsequent study, corroborated this finding, showing that the majority of a sample of people recruited on the basis of visual aphantasia also reported having reduced auditory imagery. However, this self-reported reduction in auditory imagery was not evident in performance on tasks thought to require auditory imagery, including a musical pitch imagery and voice recognition task.

A 2022 study estimated the prevalence of aphantasia among the general population by screening undergraduate students and people from an online crowdsourcing marketplace through the Vividness of Visual Imagery Questionnaire. They found that 0.8% of the population was unable to form visual mental images, and 3.9% of the population was either unable to form mental images or had dim or vague mental imagery. Sitek and Konieczna have shown that its progressive form may be a harbinger of dementia. A group of authors interviewed people with aphantasia about their lives and found that they generated fewer episodic details than controls for both past and future events, indicating that visual imagery is an important cognitive tool for dynamic retrieval and recombination of episodic details.

There have been various approaches to find a general theory of aphantasia or incorporate it into current , and research. Blomkvist has suggested that aphantasia is best explained as a malfunction of processes in the episodic system and sees it as an episodic system condition. Nanay has argued that at least some instances of this condition can be explained in terms of unconscious mental imagery. Alternative explanations for aphantasia have also been proposed in the scientific literature. Lorenzatti provides a summary of these views. Aphantasia also has been studied from philosophical perspectives. Šekrst proposed that a gradual range of perceptions and mental images, from aphantasia to hyperphantasia, influences philosophical analysis of mental imagery from a standpoint, along with influence on linguistics and . Whiteley argues that a modified theory of has to incorporate aphantasia, by involving the claim that dreams are a non-voluntary form of imagination. Additionally, research by Boran into romantic desire has shown a potential link between vividness of and romantic feelings, suggesting that mental imagery may also play a role in emotional memory and relationships.

In 2024, a research team led by Jonathan Rhodes from the University of Plymouth assessed the imagery abilities of over 300 athletes finding a small sample of 27 who had aphantasia or low imagery abilities. The researchers developed a training program over six weeks to improve imagery ability, finding that it can be significantly improved for the majority of participants. In addition, the research of Keogh and Pearson's follow-up with over 50 participants further confirmed the absence of sensory imagery in aphantasia, adding evidence to the field of study. Zeman also proposes that alterations in connectivity between the frontoparietal and visual networks may provide the neural substrate for extreme variations in visual imagery.


Notable people with aphantasia
  • , American video game designer and programmer
  • , co-founder of and former president of Walt Disney Animation Studios. Catmull surveyed 540 colleagues from Pixar about their mental visualization and found that the production managers tended to have stronger visualizations than the artists.
  • , 20th-century American theologian and philosopher. This may have influenced his development of an that did not rely on physical sensation but on rational propositional revelation from the Bible.
    (2025). 9781532607240, Wipf and Stock.
  • Laura Kate Dale, English writer and activist
  • , professional eSports player
  • , American author, educator, and YouTube influencer
  • James Harkin, British podcaster and television writer
  • , British comedian and podcaster
  • , American magician and television personality
  • , animator, author, and illustrator
  • Lynne Kelly, writer on and memory techniques. Kelly has reported that she has aphantasia, but notes that she still uses personal memory methods, such as the , which are typically thought to rely on visual memory.
  • Mark Lawrence, fantasy author
  • Yoon Ha Lee, science fiction author
  • , comedianRichard Herring's Leicester Square Theatre Podcast Book Club 17, 8th July 2022
  • , founder and the CEO of
  • , podcast host
  • , British philosopher. His aphantasia may have influenced his longtime interest in photography.
  • , academic, essayist, and poet
  • , co-creator of the web browser . In April 2016, Ross published an essay describing his own aphantasia and his realization that not everyone experiences it. The essay gained wide circulation on social media and in a variety of news sources.
  • , fantasy author
  • , journalist and co-founder of Vox


See also
  • Charcot–Wilbrand syndrome


Further reading


External links
Page 1 of 1
1
Page 1 of 1
1

Account

Social:
Pages:  ..   .. 
Items:  .. 

Navigation

General: Atom Feed Atom Feed  .. 
Help:  ..   .. 
Category:  ..   .. 
Media:  ..   .. 
Posts:  ..   ..   .. 

Statistics

Page:  .. 
Summary:  .. 
1 Tags
10/10 Page Rank
5 Page Refs