Sweetness is a basic taste most commonly perceived when eating foods rich in sugars. Sweet tastes are generally regarded as pleasurable. In addition to sugars like sucrose, many other chemical compounds are sweet, including , , and . Some are sweet at very low concentrations, allowing their use as non-caloric . Such Sugar substitute include saccharin, aspartame, sucralose and stevia. Other compounds, such as miraculin, may alter perception of sweetness itself.
The perceived intensity of sugars and high-potency sweeteners, such as aspartame and neohesperidin dihydrochalcone, are heritable, with gene effect accounting for approximately 30% of the variation.
The chemoreceptor basis for detecting sweetness, which varies between both individuals and species, has only begun to be understood since the late 20th century. One theoretical model of sweetness is the multipoint attachment theory, which involves multiple binding sites between a sweetness receptor and a sweet substance.
Newborn human infants also demonstrate preferences for high sugar concentrations and prefer solutions that are sweeter than lactose, the sugar found in breast milk. Sweetness appears to have the highest taste recognition threshold, being detectable at around 1 part in 200 of sucrose in solution. By comparison, bitterness appears to have the lowest detection threshold, at about 1 part in 2 million for quinine in solution.
In the natural settings that human primate ancestors evolved in, sweetness intensity should indicate energy density, while bitterness tends to indicate toxicity. The high sweetness detection threshold and low bitterness detection threshold would have predisposed our primate ancestors to seek out sweet-tasting (and energy-dense) foods and avoid bitter-tasting foods. Even amongst leaf-eating primates, there is a tendency to prefer immature leaves, which tend to be higher in protein and lower in fibre and poisons than mature leaves. The "sweet tooth" thus has an ancient heritage, and while food processing has changed consumption patterns,Fischler, C. Attitudes towards sugar and sweetness in historical and social perspective. human physiology remains largely unchanged. Biologically, a variant in fibroblast growth factor 21 increases craving for sweet foods.
The sweetness of 5% solution of glycine in water compares to a solution of 5.6% glucose or 2.6% fructose.
A number of plant species produce glycosides that are sweet at concentrations much lower than common sugars. The most well-known example is glycyrrhizin, the sweet component of licorice root, which is about 30 times sweeter than sucrose. Another commercially important example is stevioside, from the shrub Stevia rebaudiana. It is roughly 250 times sweeter than sucrose. Another class of potent natural sweeteners are the sweet proteins such as thaumatin, found in the katemfe fruit. Hen egg lysozyme, an antibiotic protein found in , is also sweet.
+ Sweetness of various compounds cited as "Derma, 1947" in
!Name ! Type of compound ! Sweetness | ||
0.16 | ||
0.33 – 0.45 | ||
Trehalose (α,α-trehalose) | Disaccharide | max. 0.45O'Brien-Nabors, Lyn, ed. (2012). Alternative sweeteners (4th ed.). Boca Raton: CRC Press. ISBN 978-1-4398-4614-8. Retrieved 25 June 2014. |
Isomaltulose | Disaccharide | 0.40 - 0.50 |
0.53 – 0.55 | ||
0.37 – 0.76 | ||
0.6 | ||
0.65 | ||
0.74 – 0.8 | ||
0.6 – 0.89 | ||
0.77 – 1.10 | ||
1.00 ( reference) | ||
Xylitol | sugar alcohol | 1.02 (2025). 9783540732013, Springer. ISBN 9783540732013 |
1.17 – 1.75 | ||
26 | ||
40 – 300 | ||
180 – 250 | ||
200 | ||
300 – 675 | ||
600 | ||
800 to 2000 | ||
2000 | ||
Neotame | Aspartame analog | 8000 |
162,000 (estimated) | ||
188,000 (estimated) | ||
200,000 (estimated) | ||
200,000 (estimated) | ||
230,000 (estimated) |
Some variation in values is not uncommon between various studies. Such variations may arise from a range of methodological variables, from sampling to analysis and interpretation. Indeed, the taste index of 1, assigned to reference substances such as sucrose (for sweetness), hydrochloric acid (for sourness), quinine (for bitterness), and sodium chloride (for saltiness), is itself arbitrary for practical purposes. Some values, such as those for maltose and glucose, vary little. Others, such as aspartame and sodium saccharin, have much larger variation.
Even some inorganic compounds are sweet, including beryllium chloride and lead(II) acetate. The latter may have contributed to lead poisoning among the ancient Rome aristocracy: the Roman delicacy sapa was prepared by boiling soured wine (containing acetic acid) in lead pots.
Hundreds of synthetic organic compounds are known to be sweet, but only a few of these are legally permitted as food additives. For example, chloroform, nitrobenzene, and ethylene glycol are sweet, but also toxic. Saccharin, cyclamate, aspartame, acesulfame potassium, sucralose, alitame, and neotame are commonly used.
Two natural products have been documented to have similar sweetness-inhibiting properties: gymnemic acid, extracted from the leaves of the vine Gymnema sylvestre and ziziphin, from the leaves of the Chinese jujube ( Ziziphus jujuba). Gymnemic acid has been widely promoted within herbal medicine as a treatment for sugar cravings and diabetes.
On the other hand, two plant proteins, miraculin and curculin, cause sour foods to taste sweet. Once the tongue has been exposed to either of these proteins, sourness is perceived as sweetness for up to an hour afterwards. While curculin has some innate sweet taste of its own, miraculin is by itself quite tasteless.
Human studies have shown that sweet taste receptors are not only found in the tongue, but also in the lining of the gastrointestinal tract as well as the nasal epithelium, pancreatic islet cells, sperm and testes. It is proposed that the presence of sweet taste receptors in the GI tract controls the feeling of hunger and satiety.
The threshold of sweet taste perception correlates with the time of day, probably due to oscillating leptin levels in blood that may impact the overall sweetness of food. This may be an evolutionary relict of diurnal animals like humans.
Sweetness perception may differ between species significantly. For example, even among primates sweetness is quite variable. New World monkeys do not find aspartame sweet, while Old World monkeys and (including most humans) all do. Felidae like domestic cats cannot perceive sweetness at all. The ability to taste sweetness may be lost in carnivores who do not eat sweet foods like fruits, including bottlenose dolphins, , and fossas.
In 1919, Oertly and Myers proposed a more elaborate theory based on a then-current theory of color in synthetic dyes. They hypothesized that to be sweet, a compound must contain one each of two classes of structural motif, a glucophore and an auxogluc. Based on those compounds known to be sweet at the time, they proposed a list of six candidate glucophores and nine auxoglucs.
From these beginnings in the early 20th century, the theory of sweetness enjoyed little further academic attention until 1963, when Robert Shallenberger and Terry Acree proposed the AH-B theory of sweetness. Simply put, they proposed that to be sweet, a compound must contain a hydrogen bond Electron donor (AH) and a Lewis base (B) separated by about 0.3 . According to this theory, the AH-B unit of a sweetener binds with a corresponding AH-B unit on the biological sweetness receptor to produce the sensation of sweetness.
The B-X theory was proposed by Lemont Kier in 1972. While previous researchers had noted that among some groups of compounds, there seemed to be a correlation between hydrophobicity and sweetness. This theory formalized these observations by proposing that to be sweet, a compound must have a third binding site (labeled X) that could interact with a hydrophobic site on the sweetness receptor via London dispersion forces. Later researchers have statistically analyzed the distances between the presumed AH, B, and X sites in several families of sweet substances to estimate the distances between these interaction sites on the sweetness receptor.
Sweetness modifiers
The sweetness receptor
Sweet receptor pathway
Cognition
Historical theories
MPA theory
Culture
Cited
General
Further reading
|
|